Advertisement
Research Article| Volume 102, 105904, February 2023

Learning a novel rhythmic stepping task in children with probable developmental coordination disorder

Open AccessPublished:February 03, 2023DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clinbiomech.2023.105904

      Highlights

      • Children with probable Developmental Coordination Disorder have impaired stepping.
      • These deficits are expressed in spatiotemporal and spectral parameters.
      • Cognitive concurrent tasks exacerbate these limitations.
      • This population shows improvements after a rhythmic visual cueing intervention.

      Abstract

      Background

      Developmental coordination disorder affects approximately 6% of children, interfering with participation in physical activity and can persist through adulthood. However, no studies have investigated the neuromotor mechanisms of learning of a novel task with rhythmic cueing.

      Methods

      Movement Assessment Battery for Children-2nd edition was used to identify 48 children with probable developmental coordination disorder (13.9 ± 0.05 yrs., 27% male) and 37 typically developed (13.9 ± 0.10 yrs., 54% male). While instrumented with an inertial measurement unit, both groups performed a novel rhythmic stepping task and with a concurrent auditory stroop test (dual-task), underwent seven weeks of intervention with step training with rhythmic cuing and were tested for retention five weeks post-intervention.

      Findings

      Initially, the group with probable developmental coordination disorder had a higher variability of step timing (coefficient of variation: 0.08 ± 0.003-typically developed – 0.09 ± 0.004-probable developmental coordination disorder, p < 0.05) and a frequency of peak power spectral density further from the target 0.5 Hz (0.50 ± 0.002 Hz-typically developed – 0.51 ± 0.003 Hz-probable developmental coordination disorder, p < 0.05), and were more affected by the dual-task: power spectral density at 0.5 Hz (−7.2 ± 3.3%-typically developed – -13.4 ± 4.6%- prob_DCD, p < 0.05) and stroop test errors (6.4 ± 1.1%-typically developed – -11.1 ± 2.4%- probable developmental coordination disorder, p < 0.05). The intervention led to similar improvements in both groups in coefficient of variation of step timing (0.12 ± 0.01-Pre – 0.07 ± 0.002-Post, p < 0.05), frequency of the peak power spectral density (0.51 ± 0.005 Hz-Pre – 0.50 ± 0.001 Hz-Post, p < 0.05) and relative power spectral density bandpower (3.2 ± 0.2%-Pre – 5.9 ± 0.3%-Post, p < 0.05). All improvements were retained after five weeks post-training.

      Interpretation

      Rhythmic cueing shows strong promise for enhancing motor learning in children with probable developmental coordination disorder.

      Trial registration

      Retrospectively registered on ClinicalTrials.gov with reference: NCT03150784

      Keywords

      1. Introduction

      Developmental coordination disorder (DCD) is a neurodevelopmental condition that affects approximately 6% of children (
      • Blank R.
      • Smits-Engelsman B.
      • Polatajko H.
      • Wilson P.
      • Academy European
      • for Childhood D.
      European Academy for Childhood disability (EACD): recommendations on the definition, diagnosis and intervention of developmental coordination disorder (long version).
      ). Despite having normal cognitive capabilities and opportunities to learn new motor skills, children with DCD show limited motor skill acquisition and execution in a multitude of tasks, from sporting activities to activities of daily living (
      • Battle D.E.
      Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM).
      ;
      • Gueze R.H.
      • Jongmans M.J.
      • Schoemaker M.M.
      • Smits-Engelsman B.C.
      Clinical and research diagnostic criteria for developmental coordination disorder: a review and discussion.
      ;
      • Schoemaker M.M.
      • Smits-Engelsman B.C.
      Is treating motor problems in DCD just a matter of practice and more practice?.
      ;
      • Smits-Engelsman B.C.M.
      • Niemeijer A.S.
      • Van Galen G.P.
      Fine motor deficiencies in children diagnosed as DCD based on poor grapho-motor ability.
      ). Consequently, these children tend to avoid engaging in physical activities (
      • Batey C.A.
      • Missiuna C.A.
      • Timmons B.W.
      • Hay J.A.
      • Faught B.E.
      • Cairney J.
      Self-efficacy toward physical activity and the physical activity behavior of children with and without developmental coordination disorder.
      ;
      • Bo J.
      • Lee C.M.
      Motor skill learning in children with developmental coordination disorder.
      ;
      • Rivilis I.
      • Hay J.
      • Cairney J.
      • Klentrou P.
      • Liu J.
      • Faught B.E.
      Physical activity and fitness in children with developmental coordination disorder: a systematic review.
      ). This withdrawal from a more active lifestyle can have a deeper impact throughout the individual's life. These motor deficits persist through adulthood, potentially affecting mental health and emotional regulation (p.e. anxiety, problems with personal adjustment, inattention and hyperactivity), the ability to perform daily tasks, such as driving and overall health (
      • Cousins M.
      • Smyth M.M.
      Developmental coordination impairments in adulthood.
      ;
      • Kirby A.
      • Williams N.
      • Thomas M.
      • Hill E.L.
      Self-reported mood, general health, wellbeing and employment status in adults with suspected DCD.
      ;
      • Lee D.
      • Psotta R.
      • Vagaja M.
      Motor skills interventions in children with developmental coordination disorder: a review study.
      ;
      • Lee K.
      • Kim Y.H.
      • Lee Y.
      Correlation between motor coordination skills and emotional and behavioral difficulties in children with and without developmental coordination disorder.
      ),
      Although the mechanisms that underpin this neurological condition are still largely unknown, a few typical characteristics can be often identified among these children. For example, children with DCD often show deficits in the neuromuscular control and limited visual tracking that may interfere with motor planning (
      • Johnston L.M.
      • Burns Y.R.
      • Brauer S.G.
      • Richardson C.A.
      Differences in postural control and movement performance during goal directed reaching in children with developmental coordination disorder.
      ;
      • Licari M.K.
      • Reynolds J.E.
      • Tidman S.
      • Ndiaye S.
      • Sekaran S.N.
      • Reid S.L.
      • et al.
      Visual tracking behaviour of two-handed catching in boys with developmental coordination disorder.
      ;
      • Wann J.P.
      • Mon-Williams M.
      • Rushton K.
      Postural Control and Co-ordination Disorders: The Swinging Room Revisited.
      ). This may corroborate the observations of impaired feedforward control and increased variability in motor commands in this population (
      • Cignetti F.
      • Vaugoyeau M.
      • Fontan A.
      • Jover M.
      • Livet M.O.
      • Hugonenq C.
      • et al.
      Feedforward motor control in developmental dyslexia and developmental coordination disorder: does comorbidity matter?.
      ;
      • Golenia L.
      • Bongers R.M.
      • van Hoorn J.F.
      • Otten E.
      • Mouton L.J.
      • Schoemaker M.M.
      Variability in coordination patterns in children with developmental coordination disorder (DCD).
      ;
      • Wilson P.H.
      • Ruddock S.
      • Smits-Engelsman B.
      • Polatajko H.
      • Blank R.
      Understanding performance deficits in developmental coordination disorder: a meta-analysis of recent research.
      ).
      These motor control deficits not only affect motor performance, but can also interfere with motor learning. For example, while learning a new motor skill, typically developed (TD) children require an initial higher cognitive load that shifts towards increased automaticity and reduced cognitive demand with learning (
      • Clark D.J.
      Automaticity of walking: functional significance, mechanisms, measurement and rehabilitation strategies.
      ;
      • Krajenbrink H.
      • van Abswoude F.
      • Vermeulen S.
      • van Cappellen S.
      • Steenbergen B.
      Motor learning and movement automatization in typically developing children: the role of instructions with an external or internal focus of attention.
      ). In contrast, children with DCD tend to express greater variability in their motor control and often require greater cortical and sub-cortical load while learning a new task (
      • Debrabant J.
      • Gheysen F.
      • Caeyenberghs K.
      • Van Waelvelde H.
      • Vingerhoets G.
      Neural underpinnings of impaired predictive motor timing in children with developmental coordination disorder.
      ;
      • Mackenzie S.J.
      • Getchell N.
      • Deutsch K.
      • Wilms-Floet A.
      • Clark J.E.
      • Whitall J.
      Multi-limb coordination and rhythmic variability under varying sensory availability conditions in children with DCD.
      ;
      • Zwicker J.G.
      • Missiuna C.
      • Harris S.R.
      • Boyd L.A.
      Brain activation of children with developmental coordination disorder is different than peers.
      ). Furthermore, this population shows limited capacity to accomplish this shift (
      • Debrabant J.
      • Gheysen F.
      • Caeyenberghs K.
      • Van Waelvelde H.
      • Vingerhoets G.
      Neural underpinnings of impaired predictive motor timing in children with developmental coordination disorder.
      ;
      • Mackenzie S.J.
      • Getchell N.
      • Deutsch K.
      • Wilms-Floet A.
      • Clark J.E.
      • Whitall J.
      Multi-limb coordination and rhythmic variability under varying sensory availability conditions in children with DCD.
      ;
      • Zwicker J.G.
      • Missiuna C.
      • Harris S.R.
      • Boyd L.A.
      Brain activation of children with developmental coordination disorder is different than peers.
      ), relying more on alternate strategies and feedback control (
      • Clark D.J.
      Automaticity of walking: functional significance, mechanisms, measurement and rehabilitation strategies.
      ;
      • Wilson P.H.
      Cognitive and neuroimaging findings in developmental coordination disorder: new insights from a systematic review of recent research.
      ).
      Dual-tasks (motor task and a concurrent cognitive task) have often been used to probe for the automaticity of motor learning (
      • Remy F.
      • Wenderoth N.
      • Lipkens K.
      • Swinnen S.P.
      Dual-task interference during initial learning of a new motor task results from competition for the same brain areas.
      ;
      • Schott N.
      • El-Rajab I.
      • Klotzbier T.
      Cognitive-motor interference during fine and gross motor tasks in children with developmental coordination disorder (DCD).
      ). Considering the aforementioned limitations, it becomes logical to expect that children with DCD have a more compromised ability to shift towards automatic control under conditions with concurrent cognitive loading (
      • Schott N.
      • El-Rajab I.
      • Klotzbier T.
      Cognitive-motor interference during fine and gross motor tasks in children with developmental coordination disorder (DCD).
      ;
      • Tsai C.L.
      • Pan C.Y.
      • Cherng R.J.
      • Wu S.K.
      Dual-task study of cognitive and postural interference: a preliminary investigation of the automatization deficit hypothesis of developmental co-ordination disorder.
      ). This may also reflect a limited attention resource devoted to motor processing, translating into the observed additional deterioration in neuromuscular control and motor performance (
      • Cherng R.J.
      • Liang L.Y.
      • Chen Y.J.
      • Chen J.Y.
      The effects of a motor and a cognitive concurrent task on walking in children with developmental coordination disorder.
      ;
      • Laufer Y.
      • Ashkenazi T.
      • Josman N.
      The effects of a concurrent cognitive task on the postural control of young children with and without developmental coordination disorder.
      ). In different clinical populations, rhythmic cueing has been used to improve motor processing and performance through enhancing cortical and subcortical connectivity associated with the cerebellum (
      • Braunlich K.
      • Seger C.A.
      • Jentink K.G.
      • Buard I.
      • Kluger B.M.
      • Thaut M.H.
      Rhythmic auditory cues shape neural network recruitment in Parkinson’s disease during repetitive motor behavior.
      ;
      • Roerdink M.
      • Lamoth C.J.
      • van Kordelaar J.
      • Elich P.
      • Konijnenbelt M.
      • Kwakkel G.
      • et al.
      Rhythm perturbations in acoustically paced treadmill walking after stroke.
      ). Although there is some evidence suggesting that children with DCD potentially have deficits in cerebellar function (
      • Schott N.
      • El-Rajab I.
      • Klotzbier T.
      Cognitive-motor interference during fine and gross motor tasks in children with developmental coordination disorder (DCD).
      ;
      • Tsai C.L.
      • Pan C.Y.
      • Cherng R.J.
      • Wu S.K.
      Dual-task study of cognitive and postural interference: a preliminary investigation of the automatization deficit hypothesis of developmental co-ordination disorder.
      ), training motor coordination in these children can improve psychological factors and lead to acquisition of novel motor skills, especially with task-oriented interventions (
      • Lucas B.R.
      • Elliott E.J.
      • Coggan S.
      • Pinto R.Z.
      • Jirikowic T.
      • McCoy S.W.
      • et al.
      Interventions to improve gross motor performance in children with neurodevelopmental disorders: a meta-analysis.
      ;
      • Mohammadi Oranghi B.
      • Yaali R.
      • Shahrzad N.
      The effect of eight weeks aerobic rhythmic exercises with music on motor proficiency, anxiety and depression in children with developmental coordination disorder.
      ;
      • Roche R.
      • Viswanathan P.
      • Clark J.E.
      • Whitall J.
      Children with developmental coordination disorder (DCD) can adapt to perceptible and subliminal rhythm changes but are more variable.
      ;
      • Schoemaker M.M.
      • Niemeijer A.S.
      • Reynders K.
      • Smits-Engelsman B.C.
      Effectiveness of neuromotor task training for children with developmental coordination disorder: a pilot study.
      ;
      • Schoemaker M.M.
      • Smits-Engelsman B.C.
      Is treating motor problems in DCD just a matter of practice and more practice?.
      ;
      • Smits-Engelsman B.C.
      • Blank R.
      • van der Kaay A.C.
      • Mosterd-van der Meijs R.
      • Vlugt-van den Brand E.
      • Polatajko H.J.
      • et al.
      Efficacy of interventions to improve motor performance in children with developmental coordination disorder: a combined systematic review and meta-analysis.
      ). However, to the best of our knowledge, no studies have investigated how children with probable DCD adapt their mechanisms of neuromotor control to learn a novel motor task, nor if training a novel motor task with rhythmic cueing can lead to motor learning, improvements in motor performance and enhanced automaticity.
      To address these concerns this study investigated: 1) the motor performance of probable DCD (prob_DCD) and TD children on a novel rhythmic stepping task; 2) how motor learning is experienced by each group as a function of seven weeks of a multimodal step training intervention incorporating the novel rhythmic stepping task; and 3) if increased cognitive loading through a dual-task condition can affect the rhythmic stepping motor performance of prob_DCD and TD children.
      We hypothesize that although children with prob_DCD will show overall lower motor performance with increased variability in the novel rhythmic stepping task than those in the TD group, they will lead to similar improvements to the TD group, and these changes will be retained after five weeks post intervention. Furthermore, we anticipate that the overall poorer motor competence of the prob_DCD group will be exacerbated by the dual-task condition.

      2. Methods

      This study used a non-randomised trial design, where participants executed a novel rhythmic stepping task in two conditions: rhythmic stepping (single-task) and dual-task (while performing an auditory stroop task). In addition, the participants performed the auditory stroop task while standing quietly. The three tasks were completed at baseline (pre), after seven weeks of intervention (post) and after five weeks post-training (follow-up).

      2.1 Participants

      A total of 1174 ninth year students from three community mainstream schools in the Oxford (UK) area were screened for coordination and fitness measures using the Movement Assessment Battery for Children-2nd edition (MABC-2) (
      • Henderson S.E.
      • Sugden D.A.
      • Barnett A.L.
      Movement Assessment Battery for Children-2.
      ) and the shuttle-run test (
      • Leger L.A.
      • Mercier D.
      • Gadoury C.
      • Lambert J.
      The multistage 20 metre shuttle run test for aerobic fitness.
      ).
      Participants were included if they presented: 1) age between 12 and 14 years old and 2) fitness level in the lowest quartile to control for fitness level. Furthermore, if participants had 3) poor motor skill acquisition and execution (
      • WHO
      International Classification of Diseases.
      ), they were also included to be allocated to the children with probable DCD group. Considering the identification of children with probable DCD was only based on the outcome of the MABC-2 and no other criteria, the term ‘probable DCD’ was used to classify this population (
      • Smits-Engelsman B.
      • Schoemaker M.
      • Delabastita T.
      • Hoskens J.
      • Geuze R.
      Diagnostic criteria for DCD: past and future.
      ). The exclusion criteria for all participants consisted of: 1) teacher's reported cognitive impairment; 2) behavioural/intellectual issues that would prevent safe participation or put the participant, investigators and others at risk; 3) contraindications to perform maximal exercise or physical training; 4) muscular/neurological degenerative conditions; and 5) surgery in the previous 6 months. All participants and parents/guardians provided written consent approved by the University Research Ethics Committee of Oxford Brookes University (Registration No: 161033) in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.
      The initial screening identified 85 children in the lowest quartile of fitness. Of these, 48 (13.9 ± 0.05 yrs., 27.1% male) also showed impaired motor skill acquisition and execution per their mABC-2 scores ≤15th percentile (
      • WHO
      International Classification of Diseases.
      ) and were allocated into the probable Developmental Coordination Disorder (prob_DCD) group. The remaining 37 (13.9 ± 0.10 yrs., 54.1% male) make the typically developed (TD) group (Table 1) with a mABC-2 scores >15th percentile.
      Table 1Participant demographics for typically developed (TD) and children with probable Developmental Coordination Disorder (prob_DCD).
      TDprob_DCD
      (n = 37, 54.1% male)(n = 48, 27.1% male)
      Age (years)13.9 (0.1)13.9 (0.05)
      Height (m)1.65 (0.02)1.60 (0.02)
      Weight (kg)58.4 (2.1)60.6 (2.0)
      BMI21.4 (0.6)24.1 (1.1)
      MABC-2 scores33.5 (2.5)5.5 (0.5)
      Significantly different from TD group (p < 0.05).
      Training Sessions16.3 (0.2)16.6 (0.3)
      Data shown as Mean (SEM). MABC-2 – movement assessment battery for children (2nd edition).
      low asterisk Significantly different from TD group (p < 0.05).
      Due to incomplete and/or missing data, the number of participants that were analysed at each time point differed from the total number of recruited participants in each group (pre – 33 TD, 39 prob_DCD; post – 34 TD, 43 prob_DCD; follow-up – 27 TD, 33 prob_DCD).

      2.2 Assessments

      The novel rhythmic stepping task (single-task) consisted of stepping up (approximately 20 cm) onto a step stool at a fixed alternating stepping sequence that was synchronized with a visual go cue. The visual stimulus was visible on a computer screen for 1.5 s followed by 0.5 s of a blank screen, creating a fixed stepping frequency target of 0.5 Hz. This sequence repeated for the total duration of 42.5 s, including an initial 2.5 s ‘get ready’ time interval (Fig. 1). The participants were instructed to initiate stepping synchronously with the visual ‘go cue’ and to alternate the stepping foot. This allowed for 10 cycles initiated with the right foot and 10 with the left foot.
      Fig. 1
      Fig. 1Exemplar of original raw vertical acceleration (A), transposed vertical acceleration (B) and filtered vertical relative position (C) of a representative rhythmic stepping trial.
      The auditory stroop task was completed during quiet stance and consisted of listening to the words “high” and “low” at a high and a low pitch and quickly specify the pitch of the word as accurate as possible (
      • Plummer-D’Amato P.
      • Brancato B.
      • Dantowitz M.
      • Birken S.
      • Bonke C.
      • Furey E.
      Effects of gait and cognitive task difficulty on cognitive-motor interference in aging.
      ).
      In the dual-task condition, participants performed exactly the same protocol as in the rhythmic stepping, simultaneously with the auditory stroop task (
      • Morgan A.L.
      • Brandt J.F.
      An auditory stroop effect for pitch, loudness, and time.
      ).
      Each of the three tasks (single-task, dual-task and stroop test) were completed three times for a total of nine blocks ordered pseudo-randomly, so the first block of the testing session would be the first rhythmic stepping block.
      An inertial measurement unit (IMU, LPMS-B, Life Performance Research, Japan) was used to record tri-axial accelerometry during the rhythmic stepping and dual-tasks. The IMU was fixed with adhesive tape over the participant's fourth lumbar vertebra (L4) to emulate the motion of the participant's centre of mass (CoM). The device collected data at a sampling frequency of at 100 Hz and it was positioned so the X, Y and Z axis would correspond to the antero-posterior (AP), medio-lateral (ML) and vertical directions (
      • Esser P.
      • Dawes H.
      • Collett J.
      • Howells K.
      IMU: inertial sensing of vertical CoM movement.
      ).

      2.3 Intervention

      The present study was part of a larger project where the enrolled participants underwent a multimodal exercise intervention. Specifically, both groups participated in an exercise intervention twice a week for seven weeks, while wearing an accelerometer in the dominant wrist sampling at 100 Hz (AX3, Axivity, UK). Each session included an initial warm-up, 20 min moderate to vigorous aerobic physical activity (MVPA) (50–85% predicted maximum heart rate), 30 min of personalised strength training for upper and lower limbs.
      In addition, a stepping training was also conducted, consisting of four blocks separated by 20s rest breaks: ‘slow’ (fixed alternating stepping initiation, starting at 0.4 Hz cycles and increasing to 0.49 on week seven); ‘attention’ (fixed-random sequence of stepping initiation, starting at 0.4 Hz cycles and increasing to 0.53 Hz at week seven); ‘assessment’ (fixed alternation stepping initiation, 0.5 Hz cycle throughout whole intervention); and ‘medium’ (starting at 0.44 Hz cycles on week two and increasing to 0.54 Hz in week seven).
      During the five weeks after the intervention, up-to the ‘follow-up’ testing session, the participants were instructed to live their lives normally.

      2.4 Data analyses

      This study used custom LabView2015 (National Instruments, Newbury, UK) and MATLAB (Mathworks, MA, USA) programs to analyse the collected data and calculate the main outcome variables defined and detailed below.
      Three-dimensional linear acceleration in the global reference frame was recorded from the IMU dataset. Similar to previous reports, the transposed linear acceleration was filtered using a 4th order Butterworth band-pass filter (0.2-15 Hz) (
      • Chidean M.I.
      • Barquero-Perez O.
      • Goya-Esteban R.
      • Sanchez Sixto A.
      • de la Cruz Torres B.
      • Naranjo Orellana J.
      • et al.
      Full band spectra analysis of gait acceleration signals for peripheral arterial disease patients.
      ;
      • Shahabpoor E.
      • Pavic A.
      Estimation of vertical walking ground reaction force in real-life environments using single IMU sensor.
      ). The filtered acceleration was used to integrate velocity and relative position using a mathematical approach previously described in detail (Fig. 1) (
      • Esser P.
      • Dawes H.
      • Collett J.
      • Howells K.
      IMU: inertial sensing of vertical CoM movement.
      ).
      Due to limitations inherent to the use of the accelerometer on the wrist, the intervention dataset was used to calculate the acceleration single vector magnitude (SVM) corrected for gravity by the following equation:
      SVMg=ax2+ay2+az21


      The step incidence was defined as the total number of peaks in the relative vertical position data, corresponding to when the participants reached the top of the step stool. Whereas, step timing was defined as the time interval between every relative vertical position peak.
      ML weight-transfer magnitude consisted of the difference between the right and left IMU maximum relative positions in each step-up.
      Jerk, the third derivative of position and a measure of smoothness of motion, was calculated as the change in the filtered vertical acceleration from the instant immediately prior to step-up (IMU elevation) and the instant where the IMU reaches the vertical relative peak position. Considering that duration and length of the movement can affect the magnitude of jerk, we normalized it using the following Eq. (
      • Aboelnasr E.A.
      • Hegazy F.A.
      • Altalway H.A.
      Kinematic characteristics of reaching in children with hemiplegic cerebral palsy: a comparative study.
      ;
      • Osumi M.
      • Sumitani M.
      • Abe H.
      • Otake Y.
      • Kumagaya S.I.
      • Morioka S.
      Kinematic evaluation for impairment of skilled hand function in chemotherapy-induced peripheral neuropathy.
      ;
      • van Kordelaar J.
      • van Wegen E.
      • Kwakkel G.
      Impact of time on quality of motor control of the paretic upper limb after stroke.
      ):
      Normalized Jerk=1/2TstartTendJerk2tdtduration5/length2


      Spectral analysis was performed by applying a 256 point Fast Fourier Transform (FFT) to the filtered acceleration in the time domain and Power spectral density (PSD) was calculated using Welch's method and periodic Hanning windows with a 50% overlap (
      • Ly Q.T.
      • Ardi Handojoseno A.M.
      • Gilat M.
      • Nguyen N.
      • Rifai C.
      • Tran Y.
      • et al.
      Identifying montages that best detect the electroencephalogram power spectrum alteration during freezing of gait in Parkinson’s disease patients.
      ).
      Considering that during the assessment sessions the stepping task had a target frequency of 0.5 Hz, the frequency of the peak PSD was defined as the frequency where the peak PSD occurs between 0.2 and 0.8 Hz.
      Relative PSD bandpower was calculated as the proportion of PSD within the band closely surrounding the target frequency 0.5 Hz (0.48 Hz–0.52 Hz), and expressed as a percentage of the total PSD of the full spectrum.
      For each outcome variable, the results from the dual-task were calculated as the difference between dual and single-task (rhythmic stepping) and expressed as a percentage.
      Stroop test error was calculated as the percent difference in auditory stroop task errors between performing it during dual-task and quiet standing conditions.

      2.5 Statistical analyses

      Group and time main effects, and group*time interactions were determined with a repeated-measures linear mixed-effects model, where Groups (TD and prob_DCD) and Time (pre, post and follow-up) were set as fixed factors and gender as a covariate (SPSS v22, IBM, Armonk, NY). An identical secondary analysis was also performed for the training data, where Time was composed of the different training sessions. Post-hoc calculations in all models used Sidak correction for multiple comparisons. Significance was set at p < 0.05.

      3. Results

      3.1 Rhythmic stepping

      Main effects and interactions for the main outcome variables during the single-task condition are shown in detail in Table 2.
      Table 2Summary of descriptive statistics for typically developed (TD) and children with probable Developmental Coordination Disorder (prob_DCD) groups at Pre, Post and Follow-up, and statistical analyses of Group and Time main effects and Group x Time interactions.
      GroupsPre (TD, n = 33; prob_DCD, n = 39)Post (TD, n = 34; prob_DCD, n = 43)Follow-up (TD, n = 27; prob_DCD, n = 33)GroupTimeGroup x Time
      FpFpFp
      Step incidenceTD20.97 (0.14)20.58 (0.09)20.63 (0.09)0.580.453.90<0.05 *0.160.85
      prob_DCD20.87 (0.14)20.63 (0.11)20.62 (0.09)
      Duration of step up (s)TD0.988 (0.005)0.990 (0.007)0.997 (0.003)0.120.732.700.080.180.84
      prob_DCD0.989 (0.006)0.996 (0.005)0.998 (0.003)
      CV of step timingTD0.106 (0.009)0.068 (0.001)0.065 (0.002)5.95<0.05 *60.63<0.01 *2.230.12
      prob_DCD0.131 (0.009)0.072 (0.003)0.069 (0.002)
      ML weight-transfer magnitude (m)TD0.079 (0.009)0.076 (0.007)0.079 (0.009)1.400.240.420.660.300.74
      prob_DCD0.082 (0.008)0.079 (0.007)0.092 (0.009)
      CV of ML weight-transfer magnitudeTD0.31 (0.02)0.26 (0.01)0.26 (0.02)0.380.547.27<0.01 *0.380.68
      prob_DCD0.30 (0.01)0.26 (0.02)0.24 (0.01)
      Step up peak vertical acceleration (m.s−2)TD5.4 (0.3)3.9 (0.1)3.9 (0.2)0.060.8028.50<0.01 *3.070.05 **
      prob_DCD4.5 (0.2)3.8 (0.2)4.0 (0.2)
      Step up peak vertical deceleration (m.s−2)TD−5.01 (0.04)−3.77 (0.16)−3.82 (0.17)0.440.5128.32<0.01 *2.380.10
      prob_DCD−4.51 (0.24)−3.80 (0.21)−4.04 (0.22)
      Normalized JerkTD54.4 (8.9)46.3 (6.5)44.4 (7.9)0.530.470.470.630.700.50
      prob_DCD48.0 (6.9)47.2 (6.2)53.1 (6.7)
      Frequency of peak PSD (Hz)TD0.500 (0.004)0.500 (0.001)0.498 (0.001)2.960.094.43<0.05 *3.16<0.05 *
      prob_DCD0.518 (0.008)0.499 (0.002)0.498 (0.001)
      PSD at 0.5 Hz (dB/Hz)TD1.17 (1.43)4.21 (1.16)5.56 (0.95)0.300.597.67<0.01 *0.340.72
      prob_DCD−0.51 (1.52)4.21 (1.28)4.62 (1.47)
      Relative PSD bandpower (%)TD3.0 (0.4)5.8 (0.4)6.3 (0.4)0.520.4767.90<0.01 *0.500.61
      prob_DCD3.3 (0.3)5.9 (0.4)6.0 (0.4)
      Group data expressed as Mean (SEM). CV – coefficient of variation; ML – medio-lateral; PSD – power spectral density. * represents statistical significance (p < 0.05). ** represents p = 0.051.
      Specifically, a significant main effect for Group was observed for the CV of step timing, where the prob_DCD group had an overall greater CV than TD children (p < 0.05).
      Group x Time interactions showed that the frequency of peak PSD was greater in the prob_DCD group at pre compared to post and follow-up (p < 0.05, Fig. 2A ). Additionally, prob_DCD group's frequency of peak PSD was significantly larger than in the TD group at pre (p < 0.05, Fig. 2A). The step-up peak vertical acceleration was marginally larger in TD children compared to the prob_DCD group at pre (p = 0.051, Fig. 2B).
      Fig. 2
      Fig. 2Group x Time interaction for the frequency of peak power spectral density (PSD) (A) and step up peak vertical acceleration (B). * represents a within-group significant difference from Pre. ‡ represents a within-session significant difference from the TD group. ** represents p = 0.051 between TD and prob_DCD groups in Pre.
      Main effects for Time (pre, post, follow-up) were found for the majority of the outcomes. Namely, step incidence was significantly greater at pre, compared to follow-up (p < 0.05) and marginally achieved significance compared to post (p = 0.057). In addition, the coefficient of variation (CV) of step timing, the CV of medio-lateral (ML) weight-transfer magnitude, the step-up peak vertical acceleration, the step-up peak vertical deceleration were smaller at post and follow-up than at pre (p < 0.05). The frequency of peak power spectral density (PSD) became close to the target frequency of 0.5 Hz after training and maintained at follow-up (p < 0.05). Lastly, the PSD at 0.5 Hz and the relative PSD bandpower increased significantly at post and follow-up compared to pre (p < 0.05).
      No main effects or interactions were found for duration of step-up, ML weight-transfer magnitude, or normalized Jerk (p > 0.05).

      3.2 Dual-task

      The addition of the auditory stroop test to the rhythmic stepping task, expressed as the percent change from the motor performance observed during the rhythmic stepping condition, led to several significant main effects and interactions that are detailed below (Table 3).
      Table 3Summary of descriptive statistics of the effects of the dual-task on the stepping motor performance, for typically developed (TD) and children with probable Developmental Coordination Disorder (prob_DCD) groups at Pre, Post and Follow-up, and statistical analyses for Group and Time main effects and Group x Time interactions.
      GroupsPre (TD, n = 33; prob_DCD, n = 39)Post (TD, n = 34; prob_DCD, n = 43)Follow-up (TD, n = 27; prob_DCD, n = 33)GroupTimeGroup x Time
      FpFpFp
      Step incidence (%)TD1.53 (0.85)−0.71 (0.75)−1.27 (0.57)0.300.581.080.343.59<0.05 *
      prob_DCD−1.04 (0.66)−0.07 (0.57)−0.47 (0.09)
      CV of step timing (%)TD28.8 (8.5)3.7 (4.5)−1.8 (3.7)4.12<0.05 *7.44<0.01 *1.180.31
      prob_DCD10.1 (6.3)−4.5 (4.2)−1.1 (4.7)
      CV of ML weight-transfer magnitude (%)TD18.5 (9.5)11.7 (7.9)−12.1 (6.8)0.090.771.970.153.27<0.05 *
      prob_DCD14.5 (8.6)0.2 (5.8)11.1 (6.7)
      Normalized Jerk (%)TD−5.44 (2.32)2.78 (2.18)6.46 (1.86)0.040.846.95<0.01 *2.980.06
      prob_DCD−1.57 (2.11)5.16 (1.67)1.66 (1.74)
      Vertical acceleration prior to step up (%)TD−45.3 (18.7)−15.6 (10.5)13.8 (12.5)1.380.244.94<0.01 *1.940.15
      prob_DCD−19.6 (12.9)9.4 (14.6)1.9 (8.4)
      Step up peak vertical acceleration (%)TD−9.97 (3.03)−0.15 (4.00)0.53 (2.77)2.020.163.66<0.05 *1.980.14
      prob_DCD−1.98 (4.19)6.19 (2.57)−4.49 (2.00)
      Step up peak vertical deceleration (%)TD−5.44 (3.68)4.05 (3.62)2.57 (2.86)0.140.714.04<0.05 *0.810.45
      prob_DCD−2.44 (4.32)8.50 (6.71)−1.95 (2.51)
      Step up mean vertical velocity (%)TD−69.9 (35.1)−49.5 (23.5)−65.8 (49.4)5.82<0.05 *1.600.200.570.57
      prob_DCD−151.1 (26.5)−68.1 (32.7)−120.3 (33.3)
      PSD at 0.5 Hz (%)TD−7.10 (7.90)−8.25 (4.24)−5.87 (4.14)0.930.343.99<0.05 *2.780.07 ‡
      prob_DCD−31.28 (12.03)−7.66 (3.01)0.57 (4.75)
      Relative PSD bandpower (%)TD−18.66 (4.61)−7.04 (3.25)−1.06 (3.73)0.010.925.65<0.01 *0.480.62
      prob_DCD−15.55 (5.41)−5.64 (3.10)−6.10 (3.10)
      Stroop test error (%)TD6.56 (1.18)−0.30 (0.56)2.78 (2.81)0.030.8617.67<0.01 *2.960.06 **
      prob_DCD10.96 (2.62)−1.10 (1.20)−1.56 (0.74)
      Group data expressed as Mean (SEM). CV – coefficient of variation; ML – medio-lateral; PSD – power spectral density. * represents statistical significance (p < 0.05). ** represents p = 0.057. ‡ represents p = 0.067.
      A main effect for Group showed that TD children had a significantly larger increase in CV of step timing compared to the prob_DCD group (p < 0.05). The step-up mean vertical velocity also showed a significant main effect for Group, with greater decreases observed in the prob_DCD group (p < 0.05).
      Group x Time interactions revealed that in the pre testing session, the dual-task resulted in decreases in step incidence in the prob_DCD group and increases in TD children (p < 0.05, Fig. 3A ). In the same session, the prob_DCD group observed larger decreases in PSD at 0.5 Hz (p < 0.05, Fig. 3B) and marginally greater increases in stroop test error (p = 0.060, Fig. 3C) compared to TD individuals. CV of ML weight-transfer magnitude at follow-up significantly increased for prob_DCD and decreased in TD (p < 0.05, Fig. 3D). This decrease observed in the TD group resulted in a significant difference from pre (p < 0.05, Fig. 3D). At follow-up, step incidence was significantly decreased in the TD group (p < 0.05, Fig. 3A), and in PSD at 0.5 Hz was increased for prob_DCD (p < 0.05, Fig. 3B), compared to pre.
      Fig. 3
      Fig. 3Group x Time interaction for the effects of the Dual task on: step incidence (A), PSD at 0.5 Hz (B), stroop test error (C) and coefficient of variation of medio-lateral (ML) weight-transfer magnitude (D). * represents a within-group significant difference from Pre. ‡ represents a within-session significant difference from the TD group. ‡‡ represents p = 0.060 between TD and prob_DCD groups in Pre.
      Several main effects for Time were also observed (p < 0.05). The effects of dual-task observed during post and follow-up compared to pre were significantly smaller for CV of step timing (p < 0.05) and stroop test error (p < 0.05), and greater for normalized jerk (p < 0.05) and relative PSD bandpower (p < 0.05). Step-up peak vertical acceleration and deceleration were only greater than pre at post (p < 0.05). The dual-task effects for vertical acceleration prior to step-up and PSD at 0.5 Hz were only larger than pre at follow-up (p < 0.05).

      3.3 Intervention

      Data collected during the training sessions showed main effects for Time in CV of step timing, relative PSD bandpower and frequency of peak PSD (p < 0.05, Table 4). However, due to the large number of pairwise comparisons, the Sidak correction for multiple comparisons revealed no significant pairwise differences for frequency of peak PSD (p > 0.05).
      Table 4Statistical analyses for Group and Time main effects and Group x Time interactions for the stepping motor performance during 16 sessions of the training intervention, for typically developed (TD) and children with probable Developmental Coordination Disorder (prob_DCD) groups.
      GroupTimeGroup x Time
      FpFpFp
      CV of step timing (%)0.160.693.27<0.011.110.36
      Relative PSD bandpower (%)0.0030.9583.12<0.010.870.60
      Frequency of Peak PSD (Hz)0.040.841.90<0.050.710.77
      CV – coefficient of variation; ML – medio-lateral; PSD – power spectral density. * represents statistical significance (p < 0.05).
      A progressive reduction in CV of step timing was observed throughout the training sessions, where sessions 8, 9, 15 and 16 being significantly reduced compared to session one and session 15 also showing a smaller CV of step timing than sessions 2 and 3 (p < 0.05, Fig. 4A ).
      Fig. 4
      Fig. 4Main effect of Time during the training intervention (sessions one (S1) through session 16 (S16)) for the coefficient of variation of step timing (A) and the relative PSD bandpower (B). * represents a significant difference from S1. ‡ represents a significant difference from S1, S2 and S3. † represents a significant difference from S1 and S3.
      In contrast, there was a progressive increase in relative PSD bandpower, with a significant increase from session one being observed in sessions 9 and 15 and session 9 also having a greater PSD bandpower than session 3 (p < 0.05, Fig. 4B).

      4. Discussion

      The present study generally supported the proposed hypotheses that children with impaired motor skill acquisition and execution affecting daily function (probable DCD) identified by the Movement Assessment Battery for Children (MABC-2) criteria would have impaired motor performance in a novel rhythmic stepping task, that these deficits would be exacerbated by increasing the cognitive load during a dual-task condition and that these children could learn the task and improve motor performance with a training intervention.

      4.1 Rhythmic stepping

      Before the training intervention, children with probable DCD showed an overall limitation in the ability to keep up with the specified stepping frequency, as evidenced by greater step timing variability and having the peak spectral energy at a frequency that was further from the targeted 0.5 Hz. In contrast, at pre the TD group was already able to maintain a stepping frequency very close to the imposed target, even with a larger step-up peak acceleration. It is conceivable that deficits in feedforward and feedback control mechanisms, the reported limitations in motor competence and motor processing may have led to the observed poorer stepping performance of the prob_DCD group (
      • Cherng R.J.
      • Liang L.Y.
      • Chen Y.J.
      • Chen J.Y.
      The effects of a motor and a cognitive concurrent task on walking in children with developmental coordination disorder.
      ;
      • Cignetti F.
      • Vaugoyeau M.
      • Fontan A.
      • Jover M.
      • Livet M.O.
      • Hugonenq C.
      • et al.
      Feedforward motor control in developmental dyslexia and developmental coordination disorder: does comorbidity matter?.
      ;
      • Golenia L.
      • Bongers R.M.
      • van Hoorn J.F.
      • Otten E.
      • Mouton L.J.
      • Schoemaker M.M.
      Variability in coordination patterns in children with developmental coordination disorder (DCD).
      ;
      • Wilson P.H.
      • Ruddock S.
      • Smits-Engelsman B.
      • Polatajko H.
      • Blank R.
      Understanding performance deficits in developmental coordination disorder: a meta-analysis of recent research.
      ).
      Nonetheless, when exposed to seven weeks of a multimodal exercise intervention that incorporated training of the novel rhythmic stepping task, the prob_DCD group demonstrated a motor learning capacity at least similar to TD children. Potentially driven by the training-induced improvements in variability in the step timing and medio-lateral control of the centre of mass, both groups demonstrated a reduced error in the number of steps. The spectral analysis indicated similar findings as a function of the intervention, showing a shift in the overall spectral power towards the target stepping frequency.
      Furthermore, previous reports show significant motor training dose-response to be achieved with approximately 900 min of intervention (
      • Yu J.J.
      • Burnett A.F.
      • Sit C.H.
      Motor skill interventions in children with developmental coordination disorder: a systematic review and Meta-analysis.
      ). The analysis of the data collected during the training sessions revealed that both groups experienced a progressive improvement in motor performance that was significant, as early as session eight/nine (approx. Total 1156 steps) of the training program. This may be earlier than the reported 900 min threshold. It is conceivable that the rhythmic characteristics of the intervention used in this study elicited a stronger dose-response in this population.
      The training-induced motor learning was robust enough that was retained after five weeks post-training, with no observable trends towards pre values. These results suggest that training a novel motor task with rhythmic cueing can similarly improve motor performance in TD and children with probable DCD. Interestingly, other modalities of rhythmic cueing (p.e. auditory) can develop the activation of the cerebellum, supplementary motor area (SMA), pre-motor cortex (PMC) and basal ganglia (
      • Bengtsson S.L.
      • Ullen F.
      • Ehrsson H.H.
      • Hashimoto T.
      • Kito T.
      • Naito E.
      • et al.
      Listening to rhythms activates motor and premotor cortices.
      ;
      • Grahn J.A.
      • Brett M.
      Rhythm and beat perception in motor areas of the brain.
      ). Considering that children with probable DCD potentially have deficits in central nervous system (CNS) connectivity and function, particularly in the cerebellum (
      • Schott N.
      • El-Rajab I.
      • Klotzbier T.
      Cognitive-motor interference during fine and gross motor tasks in children with developmental coordination disorder (DCD).
      ;
      • Tsai C.L.
      • Pan C.Y.
      • Cherng R.J.
      • Wu S.K.
      Dual-task study of cognitive and postural interference: a preliminary investigation of the automatization deficit hypothesis of developmental co-ordination disorder.
      ), it is plausible that besides the observed neuromotor adaptations, training with visual rhythmic cueing elicited improvements in cerebellar function involved in the development of internal models and feedforward control, similar to what has been observed with other rhythmic cueing modalities (
      • Lonini L.
      • Dipietro L.
      • Zollo L.
      • Guglielmelli E.
      • Krebs H.I.
      An internal model for acquisition and retention of motor learning during arm reaching.
      ).

      4.2 Dual-task

      When cognitive load was increased at pre during the rhythmic stepping, through the addition of the auditory stroop test, the TD group experienced a greater increase in step timing variability, a smaller decrease in overall step-up velocity and increased the stepping error (step incidence). Conversely, prob_DCD group observed a greater reduction in the PSD at 0.5 Hz and increased errors in the stroop task. Possibly, these results are a reflection of the biomechanical strategy used by each group and a trade-off between the performances of both tasks. While, TD children may have used a less biomechanically optimized strategy to complete the task, leading to greater variability and errors in stepping, but reduced amount of errors in the stroop, the opposite was observed for children with probable DCD. Nonetheless, even with the biomechanical deficits imposed by the dual-task on the TD group, their potentially greater motor competence allowed them to better adhere to the targeted frequency and have less stroop task errors than prob_DCD participants. These results may indicative of slightly better motor automaticity in the TD children when performing this novel stepping task at pre (
      • Schott N.
      • El-Rajab I.
      • Klotzbier T.
      Cognitive-motor interference during fine and gross motor tasks in children with developmental coordination disorder (DCD).
      ;
      • Tsai C.L.
      • Pan C.Y.
      • Cherng R.J.
      • Wu S.K.
      Dual-task study of cognitive and postural interference: a preliminary investigation of the automatization deficit hypothesis of developmental co-ordination disorder.
      ).
      Similar to the observations during the single-task (rhythmic stepping) condition, the training intervention generally allowed both groups to reduce motor performance deficits induced by the dual-task. Considering these results and that the intervention led to an overall improvement and optimization of the rhythmic stepping task in both groups, one could speculate that when exposed to the dual-task after training, both groups behaved more similarly to TD group's performance at pre. Hence, these results reiterate that the proposed intervention with rhythmic cueing allowed for successful motor learning and potentially increased motor automaticity, that was similarly experienced in children with probable DCD and TD children.
      Some of the limitations of this study can include the relatively small sample size. Although, main effects for Time were found for the majority of the outcome measures, some of the observed trends for the main effect of Group and the Group x Time interaction could become significant with an increased number of participants in each group. While the narrow age-range found in the ninth year of the community schools used in this study could limit the potential interpretation for DCD, this study still found several meaningful significant differences between typically developed children and children with probable DCD.
      In addition, gender had different proportions in each group. Although there is evidence that females and males do not share similar levels of motor performance in a variety of tasks (
      • Moreno-Briseno P.
      • Diaz R.
      • Campos-Romo A.
      • Fernandez-Ruiz J.
      Sex-related differences in motor learning and performance.
      ;
      • Toole T.
      • Kretzschmar J.C.
      Gender differences in motor performance in early childhood and later adulthood.
      ), it is unlikely that gender had a significant impact in the overall results, as the majority of the analysed outcomes was not affected by gender. The covariate analyses only showed significant gender effects (p < 0.05) in the rhythmic stepping condition for step-up peak vertical acceleration and deceleration, normalized Jerk and the relative PSD bandpower. For the dual-task, significant effects (p < 0.05) were only found for the step-up mean vertical velocity. Lastly, besides the step training with rhythmic cueing, the proposed training intervention also consisted of 20 min of moderate to vigorous aerobic physical activity and 30 min of personalised strength training for upper and lower limbs. Although possible, it is unlikely that these other components of the training intervention could have affected the results observed during the stepping tasks. The performance of the stepping task was strictly related to the target stepping frequency of 0.5 Hz and the participants did not train a similar movement frequency nor stepping in any of the other intervention components.
      Overall, this study may have substantial implications for clinical practice, as it has shown that rhythmic visual cueing can be helpful for children with probable DCD to learn novel motor tasks. This cueing paradigm can be incorporated in interventions to enhance motor learning in this population.

      5. Conclusions

      In conclusion, this study demonstrated that children with probable DCD identified by the Movement Assessment Battery for Children (MABC-2) criteria showed a diminished motor performance when exposed to the novel stepping task. Nonetheless, these children demonstrated, at least, a similar ability to learn a novel motor task, achieve and retain comparable motor performance as TD individuals, with seven weeks of a combined exercise and stepping training with rhythmic cueing. Furthermore, when a cognitive task was added to the rhythmic stepping, children with probable DCD showed greater impairments than the TD group, even while employing a potentially more beneficial biomechanical strategy. Similar to the training effects observed in the motor performance of the rhythmic task, the dual-task induced smaller deficits after training that were also retained. These results may help the development of interventions that target children with probable DCD to elicit motor learning and improve motor performance to levels similar to TD children.

      Funding sources

      This work was supported by the Action Medical Research and the Chartered Society of Physiotherapy who both supported this study (ref GN2445); the Elizabeth Carsson Trust and Oxford Health Biomedical Research Centre National Institute for Health Research (NIHR) Oxford Biomedical Research Centre (BRC); the Wellcome Centre for Integrative Neuroimaging is supported by core funding from the Wellcome Trust (203139/Z/16/Z); a Principal Research Fellowship from the Wellcome Trust (110027/Z/15/Z); and the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health & Human Development of the National Institutes of Health under Award Number F30HD103527. HD research is supported by NIHR Exeter Biomedical Research Centre, University of Exeter.

      Author contributions

      This statement is based on CRediT taxonomy. Authors MI, PE and HD contributed to conceptualisation, data analysis, writing and editing of this manuscript. HD is senior author and contributed to conceptualisation, resources, writing review and editing, supervision and funding acquisition. PE, AD, DS, SK, TW, HI, HJ and HD contributed to conceptualisation and funding acquisition. PE, BW, SJ and AM contributed to the investigation, project administration, data curation and writing review.

      Declaration of Competing Interest

      The authors declare that they have no known competing financial interests or personal relationships that could have appeared to influence the work reported in this paper.

      Acknowledgements

      We also want to acknowledge all teachers and members of MORES for their continuing support during in-school screening. Last but not least the participants in both screening and those selected for studies in the EPIC-study series.

      References

        • Aboelnasr E.A.
        • Hegazy F.A.
        • Altalway H.A.
        Kinematic characteristics of reaching in children with hemiplegic cerebral palsy: a comparative study.
        Brain Inj. 2017; 31: 83-89
        • Batey C.A.
        • Missiuna C.A.
        • Timmons B.W.
        • Hay J.A.
        • Faught B.E.
        • Cairney J.
        Self-efficacy toward physical activity and the physical activity behavior of children with and without developmental coordination disorder.
        Hum. Mov. Sci. 2014; 36: 258-271
        • Battle D.E.
        Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM).
        Codas. 2013; 25: 191-192
        • Bengtsson S.L.
        • Ullen F.
        • Ehrsson H.H.
        • Hashimoto T.
        • Kito T.
        • Naito E.
        • et al.
        Listening to rhythms activates motor and premotor cortices.
        Cortex. 2009; 45: 62-71
        • Blank R.
        • Smits-Engelsman B.
        • Polatajko H.
        • Wilson P.
        • Academy European
        • for Childhood D.
        European Academy for Childhood disability (EACD): recommendations on the definition, diagnosis and intervention of developmental coordination disorder (long version).
        Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 2012; 54: 54-93
        • Bo J.
        • Lee C.M.
        Motor skill learning in children with developmental coordination disorder.
        Res. Dev. Disabil. 2013; 34: 2047-2055
        • Braunlich K.
        • Seger C.A.
        • Jentink K.G.
        • Buard I.
        • Kluger B.M.
        • Thaut M.H.
        Rhythmic auditory cues shape neural network recruitment in Parkinson’s disease during repetitive motor behavior.
        Eur. J. Neurosci. 2019; 49: 849-858
        • Cherng R.J.
        • Liang L.Y.
        • Chen Y.J.
        • Chen J.Y.
        The effects of a motor and a cognitive concurrent task on walking in children with developmental coordination disorder.
        Gait Posture. 2009; 29: 204-207
        • Chidean M.I.
        • Barquero-Perez O.
        • Goya-Esteban R.
        • Sanchez Sixto A.
        • de la Cruz Torres B.
        • Naranjo Orellana J.
        • et al.
        Full band spectra analysis of gait acceleration signals for peripheral arterial disease patients.
        Front. Physiol. 2018; 9: 1061
        • Cignetti F.
        • Vaugoyeau M.
        • Fontan A.
        • Jover M.
        • Livet M.O.
        • Hugonenq C.
        • et al.
        Feedforward motor control in developmental dyslexia and developmental coordination disorder: does comorbidity matter?.
        Res. Dev. Disabil. 2018; 76: 25-34
        • Clark D.J.
        Automaticity of walking: functional significance, mechanisms, measurement and rehabilitation strategies.
        Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2015; 9: 246
        • Cousins M.
        • Smyth M.M.
        Developmental coordination impairments in adulthood.
        Hum. Mov. Sci. 2003; 22: 433-459
        • Debrabant J.
        • Gheysen F.
        • Caeyenberghs K.
        • Van Waelvelde H.
        • Vingerhoets G.
        Neural underpinnings of impaired predictive motor timing in children with developmental coordination disorder.
        Res. Dev. Disabil. 2013; 34: 1478-1487
        • Esser P.
        • Dawes H.
        • Collett J.
        • Howells K.
        IMU: inertial sensing of vertical CoM movement.
        J. Biomech. 2009; 42: 1578-1581
        • Golenia L.
        • Bongers R.M.
        • van Hoorn J.F.
        • Otten E.
        • Mouton L.J.
        • Schoemaker M.M.
        Variability in coordination patterns in children with developmental coordination disorder (DCD).
        Hum. Mov. Sci. 2018; 60: 202-213
        • Grahn J.A.
        • Brett M.
        Rhythm and beat perception in motor areas of the brain.
        J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2007; 19: 893-906
        • Gueze R.H.
        • Jongmans M.J.
        • Schoemaker M.M.
        • Smits-Engelsman B.C.
        Clinical and research diagnostic criteria for developmental coordination disorder: a review and discussion.
        Hum. Mov. Sci. 2001; 20: 7-47
        • Henderson S.E.
        • Sugden D.A.
        • Barnett A.L.
        Movement Assessment Battery for Children-2.
        2nd ed. 2007https://doi.org/10.1037/t55281-000
        • Johnston L.M.
        • Burns Y.R.
        • Brauer S.G.
        • Richardson C.A.
        Differences in postural control and movement performance during goal directed reaching in children with developmental coordination disorder.
        Hum. Mov. Sci. 2002; 21: 583-601
        • Kirby A.
        • Williams N.
        • Thomas M.
        • Hill E.L.
        Self-reported mood, general health, wellbeing and employment status in adults with suspected DCD.
        Res. Dev. Disabil. 2013; 34: 1357-1364
        • Krajenbrink H.
        • van Abswoude F.
        • Vermeulen S.
        • van Cappellen S.
        • Steenbergen B.
        Motor learning and movement automatization in typically developing children: the role of instructions with an external or internal focus of attention.
        Hum. Mov. Sci. 2018; 60: 183-190
        • Laufer Y.
        • Ashkenazi T.
        • Josman N.
        The effects of a concurrent cognitive task on the postural control of young children with and without developmental coordination disorder.
        Gait Posture. 2008; 27: 347-351
        • Lee D.
        • Psotta R.
        • Vagaja M.
        Motor skills interventions in children with developmental coordination disorder: a review study.
        Eur. J. Adapt. Phys. Activity. 2016; 9: 20-29
        • Lee K.
        • Kim Y.H.
        • Lee Y.
        Correlation between motor coordination skills and emotional and behavioral difficulties in children with and without developmental coordination disorder.
        Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health. 2020; 17
        • Leger L.A.
        • Mercier D.
        • Gadoury C.
        • Lambert J.
        The multistage 20 metre shuttle run test for aerobic fitness.
        J. Sports Sci. 1988; 6: 93-101
        • Licari M.K.
        • Reynolds J.E.
        • Tidman S.
        • Ndiaye S.
        • Sekaran S.N.
        • Reid S.L.
        • et al.
        Visual tracking behaviour of two-handed catching in boys with developmental coordination disorder.
        Res. Dev. Disabil. 2018; 83: 280-286
        • Lonini L.
        • Dipietro L.
        • Zollo L.
        • Guglielmelli E.
        • Krebs H.I.
        An internal model for acquisition and retention of motor learning during arm reaching.
        Neural Comput. 2009; 21: 2009-2027
        • Lucas B.R.
        • Elliott E.J.
        • Coggan S.
        • Pinto R.Z.
        • Jirikowic T.
        • McCoy S.W.
        • et al.
        Interventions to improve gross motor performance in children with neurodevelopmental disorders: a meta-analysis.
        BMC Pediatr. 2016; 16: 193
        • Ly Q.T.
        • Ardi Handojoseno A.M.
        • Gilat M.
        • Nguyen N.
        • Rifai C.
        • Tran Y.
        • et al.
        Identifying montages that best detect the electroencephalogram power spectrum alteration during freezing of gait in Parkinson’s disease patients.
        Conf Proc IEEE Eng Med Biol Soc. 2016; 2016: 6094-6097
        • Mackenzie S.J.
        • Getchell N.
        • Deutsch K.
        • Wilms-Floet A.
        • Clark J.E.
        • Whitall J.
        Multi-limb coordination and rhythmic variability under varying sensory availability conditions in children with DCD.
        Hum. Mov. Sci. 2008; 27: 256-269
        • Mohammadi Oranghi B.
        • Yaali R.
        • Shahrzad N.
        The effect of eight weeks aerobic rhythmic exercises with music on motor proficiency, anxiety and depression in children with developmental coordination disorder.
        Motor Behavior. 2018; 9: 57-70
        • Moreno-Briseno P.
        • Diaz R.
        • Campos-Romo A.
        • Fernandez-Ruiz J.
        Sex-related differences in motor learning and performance.
        Behav. Brain Funct. 2010; 6: 74
        • Morgan A.L.
        • Brandt J.F.
        An auditory stroop effect for pitch, loudness, and time.
        Brain Lang. 1989; 36: 592-603
        • Osumi M.
        • Sumitani M.
        • Abe H.
        • Otake Y.
        • Kumagaya S.I.
        • Morioka S.
        Kinematic evaluation for impairment of skilled hand function in chemotherapy-induced peripheral neuropathy.
        J. Hand Ther. 2017; 32: 41-47
        • Plummer-D’Amato P.
        • Brancato B.
        • Dantowitz M.
        • Birken S.
        • Bonke C.
        • Furey E.
        Effects of gait and cognitive task difficulty on cognitive-motor interference in aging.
        J. Aging Res. 2012; 2012583894
        • Remy F.
        • Wenderoth N.
        • Lipkens K.
        • Swinnen S.P.
        Dual-task interference during initial learning of a new motor task results from competition for the same brain areas.
        Neuropsychologia. 2010; 48: 2517-2527
        • Rivilis I.
        • Hay J.
        • Cairney J.
        • Klentrou P.
        • Liu J.
        • Faught B.E.
        Physical activity and fitness in children with developmental coordination disorder: a systematic review.
        Res. Dev. Disabil. 2011; 32: 894-910
        • Roche R.
        • Viswanathan P.
        • Clark J.E.
        • Whitall J.
        Children with developmental coordination disorder (DCD) can adapt to perceptible and subliminal rhythm changes but are more variable.
        Hum. Mov. Sci. 2016; 50: 19-29
        • Roerdink M.
        • Lamoth C.J.
        • van Kordelaar J.
        • Elich P.
        • Konijnenbelt M.
        • Kwakkel G.
        • et al.
        Rhythm perturbations in acoustically paced treadmill walking after stroke.
        Neurorehabil. Neural Repair. 2009; 23: 668-678
        • Schoemaker M.M.
        • Smits-Engelsman B.C.
        Is treating motor problems in DCD just a matter of practice and more practice?.
        Curr. Dev. Disord. Rep. 2015; 2: 150-156
        • Schoemaker M.M.
        • Niemeijer A.S.
        • Reynders K.
        • Smits-Engelsman B.C.
        Effectiveness of neuromotor task training for children with developmental coordination disorder: a pilot study.
        Neural Plast. 2003; 10: 155-163
        • Schott N.
        • El-Rajab I.
        • Klotzbier T.
        Cognitive-motor interference during fine and gross motor tasks in children with developmental coordination disorder (DCD).
        Res. Dev. Disabil. 2016; 57: 136-148
        • Shahabpoor E.
        • Pavic A.
        Estimation of vertical walking ground reaction force in real-life environments using single IMU sensor.
        J. Biomech. 2018; 79: 181-190
        • Smits-Engelsman B.C.M.
        • Niemeijer A.S.
        • Van Galen G.P.
        Fine motor deficiencies in children diagnosed as DCD based on poor grapho-motor ability.
        2001
        • Smits-Engelsman B.C.
        • Blank R.
        • van der Kaay A.C.
        • Mosterd-van der Meijs R.
        • Vlugt-van den Brand E.
        • Polatajko H.J.
        • et al.
        Efficacy of interventions to improve motor performance in children with developmental coordination disorder: a combined systematic review and meta-analysis.
        Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 2013; 55: 229-237
        • Smits-Engelsman B.
        • Schoemaker M.
        • Delabastita T.
        • Hoskens J.
        • Geuze R.
        Diagnostic criteria for DCD: past and future.
        Hum. Mov. Sci. 2015; 42: 293-306
        • Toole T.
        • Kretzschmar J.C.
        Gender differences in motor performance in early childhood and later adulthood.
        Women Sport Phys. Activity J. 1993; 2: 41-71
        • Tsai C.L.
        • Pan C.Y.
        • Cherng R.J.
        • Wu S.K.
        Dual-task study of cognitive and postural interference: a preliminary investigation of the automatization deficit hypothesis of developmental co-ordination disorder.
        Child Care Health Dev. 2009; 35: 551-560
        • van Kordelaar J.
        • van Wegen E.
        • Kwakkel G.
        Impact of time on quality of motor control of the paretic upper limb after stroke.
        Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 2014; 95: 338-344
        • Wann J.P.
        • Mon-Williams M.
        • Rushton K.
        Postural Control and Co-ordination Disorders: The Swinging Room Revisited.
        1998
        • WHO
        International Classification of Diseases.
        10th ed. 1992
        • Wilson P.H.
        Cognitive and neuroimaging findings in developmental coordination disorder: new insights from a systematic review of recent research.
        Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 2017; 59: 1117-1129
        • Wilson P.H.
        • Ruddock S.
        • Smits-Engelsman B.
        • Polatajko H.
        • Blank R.
        Understanding performance deficits in developmental coordination disorder: a meta-analysis of recent research.
        Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 2013; 55: 217-228
        • Yu J.J.
        • Burnett A.F.
        • Sit C.H.
        Motor skill interventions in children with developmental coordination disorder: a systematic review and Meta-analysis.
        Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 2018; 99: 2076-2099
        • Zwicker J.G.
        • Missiuna C.
        • Harris S.R.
        • Boyd L.A.
        Brain activation of children with developmental coordination disorder is different than peers.
        Pediatrics. 2010; 126: e678-e686